Perceiving perception
Professor William Mace and the
world of ecological psychology
by Jim H. Smith
|
|
|
|
Suppose
it was your job to evaluate candidates applying to become
fighter pilots. What do you imagine it takes to make a good
fighter pilot? Nerves of steel? Lightning reflexes?
Exceptional bravery? Just exactly what attributes would you
look for? Answering that question was a matter of real
significance to the Air Force during World War II. Lots of
brave volunteers were willing to become military pilots. But
which ones had what Tom Wolfe calls “the right stuff”? To
answer that question, military officials turned to
psychologists to help them explore a number of human dynamics.
One of the psychologists engaged in this research—a young
Cornell University professor named James J. Gibson—was
something of a revolutionary. It was not the candidates’
emotional make-up that piqued his interest. It was their
visual acuity. In the 1930s, he had published a study of
automobile driving. And he began his aviation research in a
similar fashion, studying how pilots took off and how they
landed.
When it came to flying a fighter plane,
Gibson knew, the ability to maneuver the plane decisively, to
make split-second decisions, could mean the difference between
life and death. As a tool for picking candidates most likely
to succeed, standard vision tests were useless. Oh, they could
tell whether potential pilots had healthy eyes. But what was
really important, Gibson realized, was not so much 20-20
vision as the capacity to quickly process information about
spatial relationships and what actually goes into that skill.
Gibson filmed take-offs and landings. He talked with pilots
about what they saw as they flew. He was particularly
interested in the all-too-frequent phenomenon of
claustrophobia and disorientation that overtook pilots when
they flew into cloudbanks. The ground and the horizon, he soon
understood, were critically important. Indeed, they were the
essential construct against which pilots maneuvered their
aircraft.
By the time Gibson’s findings were
published, in 1946, his work had led him to think about
spatial relationships in a much broader sense. It was, he
began to realize, the essence of how nearly all people
interact with, and learn from, the world around them. That
realization was the nucleus of a new way of thinking called
ecological psychology, and few have done more to advance that
field than Trinity Professor William Mace.
Living between the ground and the sky
The ecological psychology view of the
world is at once simple and profoundly complex. At its
simplest, it pivots on the adage Mace employed when he wrote
about Gibson’s landmark driving and flying studies in 1977:
Ask not what’s inside your head, but what your head’s inside
of.
“Typically, we’ve misframed the problem
of space perception,” says Mace, who joined the Trinity
faculty in 1971. “For thousands of years, the study of vision
and space has been thought of as abstract, but Gibson
understood that you need to step back from that notion and
think about the structure of light before it gets to the
eye.”
Mace articulates this perspective with a special
authority. In the world of ecological psychology, he is a
direct descendent of Gibson. The two met in 1974 when Mace
taught a summer introduction to child psychology at Cornell.
It was then, too, that he met Gibson’s wife, the renowned
psychologist Eleanor Gibson, who developed the concept of the
“visual cliff,” a tool used to study depth perception in
infants. In her study, babies who were old enough to crawl
were placed at the edge of a small drop-off covered by a sheet
of glass. Most of the babies declined to crawl forward, which
led Gibson and her collaborator, Richard Walk, to conclude
that depth perception is not learned.
Perceptual
learning, the Gibsons argued, was the essence of how humans
come to understand the world around them. And it was done
through a process called differentiation. It would be an
understatement to suggest that the Gibsons directly influenced
Mace’s view of psychology. When, in 1977, Mace wrote about
James Gibson’s work, he started by focusing on the classic
driving and flying studies. “The priority that these studies
reflect,” he said, “and which seems to have been developing
with increasing explicitness as Gibson’s ideas have developed,
is to treat perception as a biologically adaptive activity
first and as a study of ‘interesting phenomena’ much later—if
at all.”
In the years after he met the Gibsons, Mace’s
work turned increasingly toward the evolving field of
ecological psychology. By 1981, he had published more than 10
papers on various aspects of perception. That was the year he
co-founded the International Society for Ecological Psychology
(ISEP).
The
duality
How new is the field of ecological
psychology? The event that propelled the ISEP into existence
was the First International Conference on Event Perception,
which was held at the University of Connecticut in the summer
of 1981. No other conference like it had ever been held, nor
was it organized with the idea of creating a series of
conferences. But the event was so successful that Vanderbilt
University Professor Joe Lapin offered to host a second
conference, in 1983, at his school.
Meanwhile, other
conference participants had been talking for several years
about creating a scholarly journal. When a meeting to discuss
such a publication was held at Trinity, in September 1981,
Mace was one of the first to pay dues, and he became the vice
president and secretary of the new organization. It would be
several years yet before the new quarterly, Ecological
Psychology, actually emerged. But since its debut, in February
1989, it has been published every three months, and Mace has
served as its first and only editor.
Thus was born the
ISEP and, in many respects, the field of ecological
psychology. The 11 conferences since that seminal one at
UConn, 22 years ago, have confirmed the organization’s
international status, occurring in such far-flung locations as
Sweden, Italy, Holland, British Columbia, France, and
Scotland. The most recent, this summer, was at Griffith
University in Australia.
The field of ecological
psychology is still nascent, but given the range of fields to
which it can be practically applied, it is almost certain to
grow. It is instructive, for instance, to consider the ISEP’s
most recent board of directors. In addition to Mace, they
include David N. Lee, who is affiliated with Perception in
Action Laboratories, part of the Department of Psychology at
the University of Edinburgh; Benoit Bardy, who is affiliated
with the Division of Sport Sciences at the University of
Paris; Bill Warren, of the Department of Cognitive and
Linguistic Sciences at Brown University; and Carol Fowler of
Haskins Laboratories, an independent research institute in New
Haven that focuses on the biological bases of speech and
language. It is arguable that few areas of intellectual
inquiry more effectively represent the cross-disciplinary
spirit of the liberal arts. Indeed, the society’s membership
includes—in addition to psychologists—kinesiologists, artists,
roboticists, human factors engineers, and
philosophers.
“What is interesting about this field is
that it places an emphasis on understanding how organisms
function in the context of their environment,” says Jay Smart
’92, assistant professor of psychology at Miami University,
Oxford, Ohio, and a former student of Mace. “This focus drives
one to ask different questions than other perspectives would.
The questions this approach yields often deal with goals and
with how people can achieve those goals. The approach is novel
in that it asks questions at the level of behavior rather than
at the levels of cells, neurons, or organ systems. This allows
for a natural applicability to real-world problems and issues.
I see ecological psychology growing from a theory about
perception to a theory about how we function in general. I
think its appeal will be widespread, as many of the mechanisms
and principles espoused in this approach can be applied across
many academic fields, but in industrial and business settings,
as well.”
Bruce Kay ’79, who now serves as assistant
professor of psychology at the University of Connecticut, puts
it another way. “We never gain an understanding of how we
perceive the world without taking the world into account,” he
says. “It is hard for me to conceive of how we would ever
design anything for people to use without considering that
duality.” And considering the duality is precisely what
William Mace has been doing for the last quarter of a century,
as a professor, researcher, and editor. He’s been watching the
sky, keeping an eye on the horizon, studying the intricate
process by which we perceive, and thus shape, the world around
us.
back to top |